48 pages • 1 hour read
A modern alternative to SparkNotes and CliffsNotes, SuperSummary offers high-quality Study Guides with detailed chapter summaries and analysis of major themes, characters, and more.
Harris considers two perspectives on human nature: one which emphasizes the human capacity for tribalism and violence, and another which insists people are cooperative and altruistic. William Golding, author of The Lord of the Flies, believed people err towards violence like his book’s schoolboy characters. While Harris agrees a group of stranded boys would become competitive, and even violent, she believes this violence would occur between opposing groups, rather than one group picking on weaker individuals. She cites a real-life scenario in which several Melanesian boys were stranded on an island for months and lived cooperatively. She argues this was possible because the boys were close before their experience, and so operated as a cohesive unit. Harris then cites a 1954 University of Oklahoma study in which researchers established a fake Boy Scout camp in Robbers Cave State Park and divided schoolboys into two groups, the Eagles and the Rattlers. Soon after meeting, the two groups exhibited competitive behavior, calling each other names and fighting. The groups tended to be cooperative within their groups, and competitive between them. Harris argues humans are wired to perceive similarities and differences, and use this information to categorize objects, animals, and people. Human babies can categorize people based on age and gender before they even speak. One problem with categorization is that it creates a bias: People exaggerate categorized items, for better or for worse. When this instinct is applied to people, it can manifest as judgement, even sexism or racism. Within groups, people may differentiate themselves and others through behavior, physical traits, or a struggle for dominance. Families are “one of the basic and enduring types of groups,” and when there are rifts in small societies, family members tend to remain loyal to each other (126). Humans are capable of identifying with different groups (or labels) at once, such as gender or race. This tendency to form groups is similar to how many animals behave, which Harris attributes to the subconscious desire to survive and reproduce. Even though people now live in larger groups, and not exclusively with relatives, these instincts are still relevant. For example, people tend to befriend and marry people who are similar to them. Harris agrees with psychologist John Turner that people constantly self-categorize into “psychological groups” based on their current situation (132). For example, an adult among adults does not need to identify themselves as such, but they do if children enter the room. Overall, a psychological group is an essential outlet for learning norms, values, and rules.
Harris reminisces about how her own childhood shaped her personality. As a child she was loud, confident, and active, which her parents sometimes found frustrating. After switching neighborhoods and schools, she became a social outcast. Harris’s experience of being ignored for four years prompted her to become quiet, shy, and insecure. She feels this experience had a lasting impact on her development: “The children in the snooty suburb had accomplished what my parents could not: they had changed my personality” (137).
As infants, all primates are interested in engaging with other infants. In the first few years of their lives, primate infants—humans and chimps—develop an attachment to their mothers, whom they depend on for food and protection. Harris frames this as a “psychological leash” which tethers baby to mother (139). Developmentalist Mary Ainsworth tested the security of the mother-infant bond, and Harris agrees with her findings—that infants respond to their mothers depending on how these women behaved in the past. However, Harris does not agree infants use this bond as a subconscious template for later attachments. She asserts people create secure or insecure relationships with different people. Harris acknowledges mothers are important, but they cannot be a substitute for peers. She points to research which shows parental attachment is essential in a child’s early years, but only having parental relationships can be damaging. She highlights The Importance of Peer Groups through stories of abuse, captivity, and concentration camps. Harris argues these children were able to overcome extreme trauma and become healthy adults because they bonded with peers, which made them psychologically resilient.
In general, babies and toddlers are interested in interacting with each other. Traditionally, playgroups would be informally organized and multi-aged; in contemporary America, children are separated by age at school and thus have more playmates of their own age. Harris claims older children tend to prefer the company of their peers and have little contact with parents (150); thus, they tend to imitate peers. Harris believes the most important social category for children is gender, because boys and girls differ in play styles and for most of them, gender is “identifiable.” As a result, people are most socialized by peers of the same gender. “Middle childhood,” which begins when children become more intellectually advanced at age seven or eight, is particularly important: In their peer groups, children collectively decide who to look to as a leader and who to demote, which can have a lasting effect on people’s personalities.
Harris describes culture as learned behavior, and anthropologist Margaret Mead hypothesized that culture is transmitted from parent to child. In her analysis of two New Guinea groups—the Arapesh and the Mundugumor—Mead argues they employed opposing parenting strategies (peaceful and violent). Harris rejects this theory, as the supposedly peaceful Arapesh also engaged in war, but were no less gentle with their babies. She also points out that similarities in personality between children and their parents can be attributed to genetics. Likewise, some traits, such as aggression, may be due to natural selection. Unlike personality, culture is not genetic but learned. Figuring out which factors are the most important in cultural transmission is difficult, since many factors work in tandem. Harris is particularly interested in immigrant families because “[…] When the parents belong to one culture and the rest of the community belongs to a different culture, we can at least distinguish the effects of the parents from the effects of outside-the-family influence” (176).
Some children are raised in closed communities, such as Hasidic Jewish or Canadian Hutterian children, who attend schools separate from broader culture. Immigrant children tend to become “bicultural,” integrating into broader culture around peers while retaining their native language and customs at home (178). These children are “code-switchers,” changing languages depending on their social context. Over time, they become more proficient in their new home’s language than in their native language. Thus, Harris argues that neither parenting decisions nor children’s imitation of parents explains cultural transmission. In fact, some children are raised in situations in which they form a different culture from the adults in their lives. For example, there have been numerous cases of deaf children of hearing parents learning sign language against their parents and teachers’ wishes. In one case in Nicaragua, a group of deaf children spontaneously created a sign language different from their parents’ spoken Spanish, or American Sign Language. Harris concludes these children did not need to imitate adults to acquire culture, that “culture is transmitted by way of the children’s peer group” (185).
Overall, Harris argues academics should not portray children as blank slates or incomplete adults in need of adult influence—but active participants in their own cultures. Just as children establish norms in consultation with each other—such as evading authority and using taboo words—adults also have peer groups. Parents share and create norms about parenting, and are concerned about fitting in with peers. They then transmit this culture: “The children’s culture is a variant of the adult culture, and the adult culture they know best is the one they were exposed to at home. They bring that culture with them to the peer group” (197). With that said, Harris cites studies that show children who live in neighborhoods with a high rate of delinquency are at high risk of becoming delinquents themselves. However, when these children move to new neighborhoods, their behavior improves, regardless of whether they moved with their parents or not. Again, Harris laments how easy it is for socialization researchers to find correlations between parenting decisions and children’s behavior without considering other influences. She cites a study that showed children make different moral choices at home than at school or while playing sports, proving parental influence may not extend beyond the home. She reiterates that children’s culture tends to borrow from adult culture, but children alter it as necessary.
In this section, Harris connects the instinct to form groups and The Importance of Peer Groups in Socialization. Her discussion of the 1954 University of Oklahoma study on boys reveals that, when put into groups, children recognize and act on differences between groups. In spite of the children’s shared age, race, and religion, “the Rattlers became the cussing group and the Eagles stopped using cuss-words and became the praying group. The goody-goodies against the baddy-baddies” (124). This study shows children will change their personalities based on the needs and desires of their peer group. This flexibility helps them fit in with peers, thereby deepening their relationships and maximizing their chance of survival. By emphasizing the psychological significance of being a member of a group, Harris reinforces her group socialization theory—that people’s identities are shaped by their relationships with peers. The primate instinct to separate into closely-bonded groups grounds her argument in science: “[…] it is a lot easier to divide people up than to put them back together again. […] bringing [the Rattlers and the Eagles] together in noncompetitive situations—did nothing to lessen the antagonism” (118). Overall, peer relationships play a crucial role in dividing and uniting people, possibly marking them for life.
Harris reinforces Children’s Agency and Individuality and The Limits of Parental Influence through a childhood anecdote, as her ostracization by peers rather than parental influence permanently changed her personality. This story frames personality as malleable in childhood, influenced by experiences outside the home. Harris’s discussion of the brain also portrays children’s developing minds as malleable: Children do not develop rigid relationships based on the mother-infant bond but based on different levels of attachment and security with different people. Even in extreme cases, survivors of trauma can find lasting security in peers: Harris mentions two Czech boys who were abused for years, but once rescued from captivity, grew into healthy adults. She attributes the boys’ resilience to their peer relationship. She then mentions developmentalist Thomas Kindermann, who studied cliques in a fifth-grade classroom and found that within each clique, members shared an attitude towards schoolwork. Whenever a child moved from one clique to another, their attitude changed to match their new group (170). Similarly, preschoolers are suspicious of food they think they will dislike, but will eat it if peers do (154). While Harris’s anecdote and examples show how easy it is for peer relationships to lead to peer pressure, these relationships also have the potential to heal.
Harris reiterates that language acquisition reveals a child’s influences: For example, the Kelloggs’ observation of chimp baby Gua and their own son Donald showed Donald learned more chimp sounds than English words during his first years of socializing. This suggests he was more predisposed to learn from Gua than his parents. Harris also uses British boarding schools to demonstrate how students learn the aristocratic dialect of English from each other, rather than their parents, with whom they have little contact. Moreover, she mentions a British linguist’s child who adopted the dialect of the Black Californian children at her daycare rather than her parents’ accent. Again, these examples reinforce children’s peer-centric wiring.
Plus, gain access to 8,800+ more expert-written Study Guides.
Including features:
Childhood & Youth
View Collection
Coming-of-Age Journeys
View Collection
Daughters & Sons
View Collection
Family
View Collection
Fathers
View Collection
Friendship
View Collection
Juvenile Literature
View Collection
Mothers
View Collection
Nature Versus Nurture
View Collection
Popular Study Guides
View Collection
Psychology
View Collection
Pulitzer Prize Fiction Awardees &...
View Collection
Science & Nature
View Collection
Sociology
View Collection